Abstract

Discussions of synchronicity tend to focus either on the meaningful content of the experience, or on speculation about possible mechanisms underlying the phenomena. The present paper suggests that the symbolic or meaningful content of some synchronistic phenomena are themselves governed by identifiable dynamics associated with the emergence of symbol systems generally. Specifically, these dynamics are associated with complex dynamical systems theory and give rise to phenomena governed by power laws such as Zipf’s law. It is suggested that synchronicities, which display distinctly symbolic features, behave in ways that conform to power-law distributions in which highly coupled systems form rare outlier aggregations referred to as "dragon kings". This terminology is explained and related to the experience of synchronistic phenomena.

:mao-shining:

  • GalacticFederation [none/use name]
    hexagon
    ·
    4 years ago

    The Scarab Beetle

    Jung’s paradigmatic example of a synchronistic event, the famous example of the scarab beetle, is well known to anyone interested in Jung’s theory of synchronicity. While Jung provided a host of other examples of phenomena that he wanted to include within the framework of synchronistic events, this case remains particularly salient and instructive. In 2008, at the Journal of Analytical Psychology conference in Italy, following the work of both Main (2004, 2007) and Bishop (2000), I argued that the entire situation Jung describes in this case required much more attention to the role of the scarab beetle in Jung’s own iconography and symbolic lexicon. Main, in particular, has commented on the various instances where scarabs are remarked on by Jung, or otherwise appear in materials that interested him, particularly in the alchemical tradition. With the publication of the Red Book we can push the significance of the scarab even further in its relationship to Jung’s symbolic world, given the appearance of several scarabs both in the text and in his paintings.

    In all, I suggested at that time that the occasion of the scarab beetle dream was in all likelihood a major transference/counter-transference event which may well have been of greater significance to Jung than it was to the dreaming patient. While Jung characterized the event as if the scarab dream was uniquely his patient’s experience of a moment of rebirth, I believe we must really take the situation more as a transferential response on the part of the patient to Jung’s own symbolic preoccupations. The sudden movement in the treatment may then be seen as much a counter-transferential response on Jung’s part as a breakthrough on the part of the patient.

    I will therefore argue in what follows that the scarab synchronicity is an instance of complex dynamics involving intense symmetric coupling within the patient-analyst system. This dynamics gave rise to a system-wide amplification of the symbolic interactions associated with the dream, resulting in a global reorganization, or transformation, of the system as a whole. This is a form of emergent phenomenon that has been called a “dragon king.” I will attempt to make good on this proposition in what follows.

    On the other hand, in my 2008 paper I also proposed that, because of the complexity of this case, the symbolism of the scarab and the response to the dream, as well as the fortuitous appearance of the chaffer beetle, created a moment of “symbolic density”. One objective in this paper is to flesh out the concept of symbolic density and show how it rests on certain features of symbolic processes in general that add to our understanding of synchronicity while at the same time removing some of its more perplexing qualities.

    Let me also say that I do not want to represent this work as my last word on synchronicity, given the variety of forms it takes in Jung’s writings on the subject. My focus will be on those instances of synchronicity that are most explicitly associated with the occurrence or generation of symbolic meaning in the present moment, leaving aside precognitive and other phenomena Jung broadly associated with synchronicity.

    • GalacticFederation [none/use name]
      hexagon
      ·
      4 years ago

      Zipf’s Law

      Had he known of them, Jung might have appreciated a set of mathematical formulations that have the important quality of describing a wide variety of phenomena with no intrinsic connection to one another. There is, if you will, something transcendental about them, in the philosophical sense of being universal conditions of the world, without reference to specific states of affairs. The ones I am particularly interested in, in relation to the symbol, are power-law distributions, particularly Zipf’s law, but also including the fractal geometry of Mandelbrot (1981, 1997) and scale-free structures of networks. Let me note that all of these patterns involve, amongst other characteristics,a relationship to scaling phenomena, or what is usually referred to as scale-invariance. This means that these patterns apply regardless of the scale at which the phenomena are analyzed.

      Zipf’s law is named after the American linguist George Kingsley Zipf (1902–1950). He was something of a polymath or a dilettante, depending on your point of view, who began by examining variations in the size of cities. He discovered that, within a given geographic area, the size of population concentrations, from small villages to large cities is governed by a so-called power-law distribution. In the case of cities the abundance (or frequency) of agglomerations of sizes followed a deceptively simple 1/s distribution.

      The outcome of this calculation looks like a graph schematically depicted in Figure 1a. However, when the results are converted to a doubly logarithmic graph (log-log plot), the chart looks like Fig. 1b, which exhibits the characteristic linearity of a power-law distribution. Zipf’s next step, and the result for which he is remembered, was to examine the frequency of words in a text. He found that the frequency of words in a text, ranked according totheir abundance, fell like 1/r as a function of their rank r. In a log-log plot, the slope of the resulting linear function is then minus one.

      In addition to his work on the statistics of word frequency, Zipf proposed a model for the generation of lexicons, or symbol systems, that he referred to as the principle of least effort. Briefly, the idea here was simply that both the listener and the speaker in an exchange of signs would seek to minimize their expenditure of energy – that is, put in the least effort (Zipf 1949). This means that a kind of negotiation would take place between the parties of an exchange, in which each sought the greatest level of understanding for the least effort.

      Needless to say, the simplest way to achieve this goal is to have a shared lexicon of exact one-to-one relations between the elements in the lexicon and the objects referenced by the lexicon. However, this approach entails massive memory requirements to insure the least ambiguity. It is the way in which most animals, other than humans, communicate. The monkey cry that designates the presence of a snake is distinct from the cry that designates an eagle. But while some animals can learn fairly large lexicons in captivity, and under well controlled conditions, we also know that in the wild the upper bound for say the bonobo chimpanzee, perhaps the most cognitively advanced primate short of humans, is on the order of about 40 “words”, with little or no syntax. These lexicons are essentially indexical rather than symbolic, in the terms used by Peirce and Deacon.

      Explicitly drawing on Deacon’s and Peirce’s distinction between symbols and indexes, Ferrer-i-Cancho and Sole (2003) simulated the development of a lexicon beyond the indexical level and concluded that Zipf’s law was not simply a descriptive tool. Rather, it was actually a necessary emergent property of symbolic systems which, they also demonstrated, exist in what is known as a phase transition – a condition such as what happens as water turns to steam or freezes into ice.

      However, the symbolic phase space in this instance has the added feature that the symbolic system proper remains in the phase space and does not resolve either into indexicals or into meaningless randomness. This feature, which entails a significant degree of referential ambiguity, was, they speculate, a likely contributing factor in the evolution of language, because it allows a limited lexicon to refer to a larger set of objects. In the presentation of their findings one can see how a phase transition emerges where the effort of the speaker becomes roughly equal to the effort of the listener Fig. 2.

      The model of Ferrer-i-Cancho and Sole (2003) created a very abstract and idealized understanding of a language or symbol system. They have gone further in other papers, to examine the emergence of syntax and also to argue that semantic content may follow Zipf’s law as well. This is a more controversial claim, but it has received some support from other researchers. For instance, Vogt (2004) adds a dimension to this discussion in that he enlarges the set of possible symbolic structures by examining the ways in which referential tokens can be aggregated into categories. Once again, the principle of least effort is at work, but the objective is to locate the category that best discriminates one reference from others.

      Vogt refers to the conceptual structures of symbolic systems in terms of their density: symbolic density. He argues that in a search for appropriate categorical structures, the principle of least effort will motivate movement through a hierarchy of increasingly dense categories. Furthermore, this hierarchy of category density can be subsumed under a Zipf-Mandelbrot power law. This is exactly my argument regarding Jung’s system; the complex, the archetype, synchronicity and even the notion of the Self are scale-invariant symbolic structures of increasing density that should, by virtue of their symbolic nature, as well as the curious fact of the scope of power-law like phenomena, fall under Zipf’s law.

      A question can be raised at this point, however. If synchronicities are simply at the high end of a power law, why do they carry the level of meaning and affective impact that they typically do? A large earthquake, after all, is, as Sornette (2003) has remarked, simply a small earthquake that keeps going. But, if the same principle applies to synchronicities, why do you havethe experience of a “rupture of time” (Main 2004)?

      Ironically, part of the answer is already available in the work of Ferrer-i-Cancho and Sole (2003), in their discussion of the emergence of language. As was mentioned, this process consists in the formation of a phase transition in which an entirely new and distinct regime emerges as symbols overwhelm the earlier simple indexical reference. This transition is, in no small measure, a catastrophe, in the technical sense of the word – and perhaps in practice as well, insofar as it likely catapulted the genus homo into an entirely different life-world to the general detriment of other organisms.

    • GalacticFederation [none/use name]
      hexagon
      ·
      4 years ago

      Symbolic Density

      Let me begin with a more general point of view on Jung regarding the importance of his early work on the word association test and on the linguistic patterns of dementia praecox [Schizophrenia]. Jung’s work in these areas is, in my experience, too easily overlooked in preference for the later materials on archetypes, typology, and the alchemical writings. It is important for our purposes in thesetwo areas that the word association test revealed important foundational elements of psychological functioning in the phenomenology of associative networks, particularly the affective content of these networks.

      As Spitzer (1992) argues in his historical review of the word association experiment, Jung significantly enlarged the scope of the test and, more importantly for psychiatry, used it to argue for the deep coherence of psychotic discourse, thereby influencing Bleuler’s model of schizophrenia. Jung’s understanding of schizophrenic discourse emphasized the presence of relatively coherent associative paths connecting the manifest elements of the discourse, but large portions of that connective network were obscured. This point of view on psychotic discourse has been investigated more recently by Rebeiro (1994) among others, who mapped the subterranean – or unconscious – networks of association that exist in the often seemingly disjointed ramblings of severely psychotic patients.

      The essential element in this early work of Jung is the centrality of associative relationships among elements that we can reasonably call symbols, or at least symbolically significant markers in the individual’s psychic world. For an example, I recommend reading Jung’s masterful unpacking of thesymbol of the linden tree in his study of dementia praecox. What I now want to do is begin to tie Jung’s work on symbolic networks to a larger body of research on the nature of the symbolic, beginning with the American pragmatist and founder of semiotics, Charles Sanders Peirce (1839–1914). Peirce divided sign systems into three essential levels, the icon, the index and the symbol. Deacon (1997), in his essential study The Symbolic Species, draws directly on Peirce’s work as part of his discussion of the emergent nature of truly symbolic systems.

      We do not need linger on icons, as they are relatively direct representations of the object of concern, such as a portrait of Louis XIV or a photograph of Jung. Where matters become interesting is in the move to the indexical level of signification. At this level, a sign begins to aggregate instances into larger sets such as monarchs or famous psychologists. There remains, however, a known, and essentially unquestioned referential relationship between the sign stimuli and the object or action in question.

      Deacon identifies a critical transitional stage to a more complex arrangement of tokens, where the tokens, still largely indexical in relation to their objects, begin to arrange themselves in patterns of token-to-token combination. Importantly, these indexical combinations of token interactions still do not relinquish their referential relationship to objects or actions – monarchs who exercized great power throughout Europe.

      As we move to the fully symbolic level, the relationships that matter shift to those between the tokens themselves, and only by abstraction to their physical or pragmatic referents. At this point one could thematize the nature of kingship, or, perhaps, the archetype of the king. Without elaborating on this in detail, I would suggest that Deacon’s approach is a useful illustration of what Jung was beginning to observe in the word association test, and in his work with dementia praecox and the discourse of the psychotics. These observations formed the foundation for what would eventually be his theory of archetypes.

      Another brief comment to set the stage for what is perhaps the most difficult part of my argument. We are all aware of the importance of Jung’s close relationship with Wolfgang Pauli and their extended discussion of synchronicity in relation to quantum physics. Pauli was, in many ways, the only interlocutor to whom Jung paid actual attention and even deference. Pauli reciprocated with an extraordinary level of engagement in topics that he was aware could easily marginalize him in some scientific circles.

      That said, when we read their correspondence, it is clear that one of Pauli’s greatest challenges in the exchange was getting Jung to understand the statistical nature of quantum mechanics. I raise this historical bit of the discussion of synchronicity because, with the advent of quantum mechanics, much of theoretical physics became statistical in nature, and that process has continued to the present. The aspect of this development that concerns us here is the application of the statistical methods developed by physicists, as well as some other disciplines such as economics, to domains beyond their normal purview, including the study of language and the nature of symbols. One way in which my own argument over the last several years might be framed is that Jung, as well as Pauli, with some of this research at hand, might have arrived at a very different understanding of archetypes and synchronicity.